是文化影響認知?還是語言影響認知?

Lucy and Gaskins (2001)有一系列的研究試圖回答這個問題。他們先讓受試者看目標物,接著會給受試者兩個選項,讓他們選擇其認為和目標物最相似的物品。一個物品和目標物的形狀是一樣,但是材質是不一樣的;另一個物品則是相同材質,但是形狀不一樣。受試者不會聽到任何新的詞語。英語母語者的選擇傾向以形狀來決定兩個物品的相似;但是來自墨西哥的一個馬雅語言尤卡特語(Yucatec Maya)人則傾向根據材質。

Lucy and Gaskins (2001) Lucy gaskin把同樣的任務讓七歲的英語和尤卡特語母語者進行,結果並沒有發現如成人的傾向,也就是不論英語或尤卡特語的小孩都傾向以形狀來決定兩個物品的相似。如果我們把兩個實驗一起看的話,七歲小孩也是能夠自由運用語言的人,但在七歲小孩身上沒發現差異,但是成人身上會發現差異,那語言影響的可能性應該就比較低,文化影響認知的可能性比較高。

回答相同的問題,Levinson (1996) 採用不同的取徑。有一些語言習慣用相對位置來描述事物的位置,有一些語言則傾向用絕對的位置,德語符合前者,Tenejapa符合後者。讓受試者坐在桌子前面,桌上依序由左至右擺著牛、豬、馬、羊四個小玩具,受試者被要求記下這個位置後,受試者向後轉(轉了180度),然後依照記下來的位置,在另一張桌上擺出來。結果,德語母語者仍然會依照自己的視角,由左至右擺了四個小玩具。但是Tenejapa卻以先前的絕對位置,由左至右擺放了羊、馬、豬、牛。

另一個實驗則讓Tenejapa面向北方看一個卡通,卡通裡的人物都從東邊往西邊運動,接著再請受試者到另一個房間報告剛剛看到的卡通。德語母語者仍保持相對位置的描述;Tenejapa則是把卡通人物剛剛出現的方向以絕對位置報告出來。

Levinson (1996) 認為,這是因為傾向絕對或相對位置的語言使人們有如此不一樣的概念表徵。

雖然,這樣子的結果似乎說明了語言影響了認知,但是這個絕對位置和相對位置的系統在許多生物身上都有,因此它是獨立於語言的認知。不論德語母語者或Tenejapa,他們肯定也有有絕對位置和相對位置的概念,只是他們的傾向不一樣。如果語言對於空間的認知有影響的話,那不會是因為語言而對空間有了新的認知,而且語言使人們選擇那個他更習慣的空間思考。

References

Bloom, P. (2000). Words and Concepts. In (), How children learn the meanings of words. The MIT Press.
Bowerman, M. & others (1996). Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective. In Peterson, L. Nadel & M. Garrett (Ed.), Language and space (pp. 385-436). MIT Press.
Carruthers, P. (1998). Language, thought and consciousness: An essay in philosophical psychology. Cambridge University Press.
Davidson, N. S. & Gelman, S. A. (1990). Inductions from novel categories: The role of language and conceptual structure. Cognitive Development, 5(2), 151-176.
De Villiers, J. & De Villiers, P. (1997). Linguistic determinism and theory of mind. , , .
Dehaene, S. (2011). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. OUP USA.
Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of minds: toward an understanding of consciousness. Basic Books.
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought (Vol. 5). Harvard University Press.
Gelman, S. A. & Coley, J. D. (1990). The importance of knowing a dodo is a bird: Categories and inferences in 2-year-old children.. Developmental psychology, 26(5), 796.
Gelman, S. A. & Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23(3), 183-209.
Gelman, S. A. & Markman, E. M. (1987). Young children's inductions from natural kinds: The role of categories and appearances. Child development, , 1532-1541.
Gentner, D. & Boroditsky, L. (2001). Individuation, relativity, and early word learning. In (), Language, culture and cognition. Cambridge University Press.
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 178.
Goodglass, H., Denes, G. & Calderon, M. (1974). The absence of covert verbal mediation in aphasia. Cortex, 10(3), 264-269.
Gopnik, A. & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The development of categorization in the second year and its relation to other cognitive and linguistic developments. Child development, , 1523-1531.
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N. & Bryant, P. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories (Vol. 1). Mit Press Cambridge, MA.
Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In (), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge University Press.
Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: Crosslinguistic evidence. In (), Language and space (Vol. 109). MIT Press.
Lucy, J. A. & Gaskins, S. (2001). Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: A comparative approach. In (), Language acquisition and conceptual development. Cambridge University Press.
Markman, E. M. & Hutchinson, J. E. (1984). Children's sensitivity to constraints on word meaning: Taxonomic versus thematic relations. Cognitive Psychology, 16(1), 1-27. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(84)90002-1
Murphy, G. L. & Lassaline, M. E. (1997). Hierarchical structure in concepts and the basic level of categorization. In (), Knowledge, concepts, and categories. MIT Press.
Perner, J., Leekam, S. R. & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(2), 125-137.
Solomon, G. E. (1997). Conceptual change and wine expertise. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 6(1), 41-60.
Tajfel, H. & Wilkes, A. L. (1963). Classification and quantitative judgement. British journal of psychology, 54(2), 101-114.
Waxman, S. & Thompson, W. (1998). Words are invitations to learn about categories. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(1), 88-88.
Waxman, S. R. & Markow, D. B. (1995). Words as invitations to form categories: Evidence from 12-to 13-month-old infants. Cognitive psychology, 29(3), 257-302.
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128.


沒有留言:

張貼留言

留個言吧…